“Unraveling the Mathematical Firestorm: What Sparked the Debate That Set the Number Crunchers Against Each Other?”
Have you ever found yourself caught in a mind-bending scenario that made you question your grasp on logic? Picture this: You’re a contestant on the iconic game show Let's Make a Deal, faced with three tantalizing doors. One of them conceals a shiny sports car, while the other two—the dreaded goats—offer nothing but disappointment. Just when you think you’ve made a solid choice, the host Monty Hall swoops in and opens one of the doors to reveal a goat. Suddenly, you’re presented with a crucial dilemma: Do you stick with your initial selection or switch your choice to the other closed door? This quirky puzzle, known as the Monty Hall Problem, was innocently tossed into the academic ring by statistician Steve Selvin back in 1975, but it morphed into one of the most heated, controversial debates in mathematics. How could a seemingly straightforward game show riddle spark such a ruckus? Buckle up as we explore this captivating tale of probability, persistence, and perhaps a dash of pride! LEARN MORE
Imagine you are a contestant on the classic game show Letâs make a Deal. The host, Monty Hall, presents you with three closed doors. Behind one is a brand-new sports car, while behind the other two is a goat.
You make your choice and prepare to discover whether you will leave the studio the proud owner of a set new ride or of a four-legged garbage disposal. But at the last moment, Monty offers to make your choice a little easier; he opens one of the doors, revealing a goat, and gives you the option to either change your guess or stick with your original choice. What do you do?
This is the Monty Hall Problem, first posed by American statistician Steve Selvin in a 1975 letter to the journal American Statistician. For 15 years after its introduction, the puzzle was discussed by a number of academic publications but failed to make much of an impact. But when in September 1990 reader Craig F. Whitaker submitted the question to Parade Magazineâs Ask Marylin column, it unexpectedly triggered one of the most heated, controversial, and downright toxic debates in the history of mathematics.
Ask Marylin, which has run in Parade since 1986, is written by Marylin sos Savant, who for many years was widely known as the âWorldâs Smartest Person.â Born in St. Louis, Missouri in 1946, in 1956 at the age of 10 vos Savant took the standard 1937 Stanford-Binet Test, achieving an unprecedented IQ score of 228. Later in the 1980s she scored a 46/48 on the Hoeflin Mega Test, which revised her IQ down to a more reasonable but still impressive 186. Based on these two scores, vos Savant held the Guinness World Record for the highest recorded IQ from 1986 to 1989 when the category was finally retired.
At first glance the answer to the Monty Hall Problem appears obvious: your chances of picking the car, originally 1 in 3, have now been increased to 1 in 2. However, as youâve been given no additional information as to which door the car sits behind, it makes no difference whether you stick with your original guess or switch; your odds of winning remain the same. However, in her reply to Craig Whittakerâs statement of the problem, vos Savant gave an altogether different answer, arguing that the best strategy was, in fact, to switch your first guess:
âYes; you should switch. The first door has a 1/3 chance of winning, but the second door has a 2/3 chance. Hereâs a good way to visualize what happened. Suppose there are a million doors, and you pick door #1. Then the host, who knows whatâs behind the doors and will always avoid the one with the prize, opens them all except door #777,777. Youâd switch to that door pretty fast, wouldnât you?â
The reaction to this counter-intuitive solution was swift and surprisingly hostile, with vos Savant being bombarded with letters from hundreds of readers – several with PhDs in statistics and related fields – staunchly refuting her analysis. These ranged from the relatively polite but dismissive:
âYour answer to the question is in error. But if it is any consolation, many of my academic colleagues have also been stumped by this problem.â
-Barry Pasternack, Ph.D.
California Faculty Association
â¦to the arrogant and condescending:
âSince you seem to enjoy coming straight to the point, Iâll do the same. You blew it! Let me explain. If one door is shown to be a loser, that information changes the probability of either remaining choice, neither of which has any reason to be more likely, to 1/2. As a professional mathematician, Iâm very concerned with the general publicâs lack of mathematical skills. Please help by confessing your error and in the future being more careful.â
-Robert Sachs, Ph.D.
George Mason University
âYou blew it, and you blew it big! Since you seem to have difficulty grasping the basic principle at work here, Iâll explain. After the host reveals a goat, you now have a one-in-two chance of being correct. Whether you change your selection or not, the odds are the same. There is enough mathematical illiteracy in this country, and we donât need the worldâs highest IQ propagating more. Shame!â
-Scott Smith, Ph.D.
University of Florida
â¦to the downright misogynistic:
âMaybe women look at math problems differently than men.â
-Don Edwards
Sunriver, Oregon
In response to this deluge of criticism, vos Savant devoted her three next columns to patiently re-explaining the logic of her solution, but the majority of her respondents remained unconvinced, with one writing nearly a year later:
âI still think youâre wrong. There is such a thing as female logic.â
So, is it possible that the âworldâs smartest person” actually got it wrong? Well, actually, no. Much of the confusion regarding the Monty Hall Problem stems from the ambiguous manner in which it was stated in Craig Whittakerâs original letter to Parade Magazine. Most of those who disagree with vos Savantâs answer assume that the hostâs choice of which door to open is entirely random, and in this case the conclusion that the contestantâs chances become 50/50 would be correct. However, in Steve Selvinâs original formulation of the problem this is not the case. After all, if the hostâs choice of door were random, there is a chance he would open the door with the prize behind it, ruining the game. Thus, the host must always open a door with a goat, and it is this detail which makes all the difference.
To understand why, imagine the three possible scenarios when playing the game: you can either guess the Prize, Goat 1, or Goat 2. If you guess the Prize, then your best strategy is to stay put, since switching will lose you the game. But if you choose Goat 1 or Goat 2, then the best strategy is to switch. As the optimal strategy in 2 out of 3 possible scenarios is to switch, your chances of winning are 2/3 if you switch compared to only 1/3 if you stick to your original guess. So your best bet is always to switch.
So how did a seemingly innocuous probability problem manage to ignite such fierce and passionate condemnation? The Monty Hall problem is what American Philosopher Willard Quine called a veridical paradox – a result that intuitively looks false but can nonetheless be logically proven to be true. Human intuition is particularly ill-suited to dealing with problems of probability, and when presented with a counter-intuitive solution a common reaction is simply to reject it outright. In the case of Marilyn vos Savant this knee-jerk incredulity – along with the egos of top athematicians and not a small amount of sexism – appear to have combined into the perfect storm of academic controversy.
Thankfully, however, history appears to have vindicated Marilyn vos Savant, with polls showing that by 1992 56% of readers and 71% of academics had accepted her solution, compared to only 8% and 35% two years before. And even Robert Sachs of George Mason University, once among vos Savantâs harshest critics, eventually wrote her to repent his former arrogance:
âAfter removing my foot from my mouth I’m now eating humble pie. I vowed as penance to answer all the people who wrote to castigate me. It’s been an intense professional embarrassment.â
Expand for References
Kaplan, Ellen and Michael, Chances Areâ¦: Adventures in Probability, Penguin Books, 2007
Crockett, Zachary, The Time Everyone âCorrectedâ the Worldâs Smartest Woman, Priceonomics, February 19, 2015 https://priceonomics.com/the-time-everyone-corrected-the-worlds-smartest/
Tierny, John, Behind Monty Hallâs Doors: Puzzle, Debate, and Answer? The New York Times, July 21, 1991 https://www.nytimes.com/1991/07/21/us/behind-monty-hall-s-doors-puzzle-debate-and-answer.html
Vos Savant, Marylin, Game Show Problem, marylinvossavant.com https://web.archive.org/web/20130121183432/http://marilynvossavant.com/game-show-problem/
The post The Story Behind the Most Heated, Controversial, and Downright Toxic Debate in the History of Mathematics appeared first on Today I Found Out.